Menu Close
Chestnut-crowned babblers provide a perfect opportunity to test theories of cooperative breeding. Enchylaena

Babblers show why birds of a feather stick together

Blood is thicker than water when it comes to being a team player – at least if you’re a bird in outback Australia. So shows a new study I was involved in, published this week in the journal Proceedings of the Royal Society B.

Our work sheds light on the evolutionary puzzle of why some individuals cooperate with each other for a common good rather than pursuing their own selfish agenda.

Why do we ever play nicely with others? The world around us abounds with weird and wonderful examples of cooperation, from the sharing of blood meals by vampire bats to self-sacrificing sperm in wood mice. In fact, some of the most successful animals on the planet, ourselves included, are highly cooperative.

Jodie Crane

One of the most extreme forms of cooperation is when individuals sacrifice their own procreation in order to care for someone else’s kids (cooperative breeding).

Being so altruistic seems to contradict Darwin’s theory of natural selection – wouldn’t they be better off being selfish? Clearly, the evolution of cooperative breeding requires some special explanation.

One theory (group augmentation) suggests such behaviour is actually self-interested: in group-living species, individuals can increase their own success by helping to make their group larger. For instance, there is safety in numbers and by raising extra group members they could – selfishly – reduce their own chances of getting nabbed by a hungry predator.

An alternative theory (kin selection) proposes that, because we share DNA with our relatives, helping family members to succeed might help to pass these “unselfish genes” on to the next generation.

In other words, promoting the welfare of your brothers and sisters, who share approximately half of your genes, helps to propagate a line of future helpers.

Enchylaena

Chestnut-crowned babblers – the subjects of our study – provide a perfect opportunity to test these two theories because babblers have a choice of who to help in their group.

The desert environment in outback Australia where they live is harsh and the extra help can mean the difference between life and death for the chicks. Most group members help to look after the chicks, despite not being the parents themselves.

But like any team activity, some individuals do the lion’s share of all the work, while others do nothing at all.

As it turns out, babblers are not swayed by group size when deciding whom to help. Instead, they seem primarily driven by an altruistic urge to help their family.

Even though they might be better off living in a larger group, they invariably choose to help their relatives instead. They also work much harder when they are looking after their brothers and sisters rather than more distant kin.

Our study took place on a wild population of babblers in red desert country at the University of New South Wales Arid Zone Research Station, near Broken Hill, NSW. The video above, from about the three-minute mark, shows both the research station and our project there.

By fitting the birds with tiny transponders it was possible to see which nests they visited and how many times they fed the chicks in order to figure out who cooperated with who.

These findings are relevant for understanding how our own societies evolved too because much of human society is underpinned by cooperation. Kinship is clearly important for babblers and we can speculate that it may have been important for the evolution of cooperation in people too.

In modern human societies, Good Samaritans can extend a helping hand to complete strangers. But our research suggests that cooperation may have started within the family.

It seems charity truly does begin at home.

Want to write?

Write an article and join a growing community of more than 182,300 academics and researchers from 4,941 institutions.

Register now